Phonological Awareness
NICHCY News Digest Volume 25,
August 1997
Interventions for Students with Learning Disabilities Author: William Ellis Reading is a complex activity.
It sends our brains into a frenzy of electrical impulses that zig and zag through
matter in ways we still do not totally understand. It organizes sights and sounds
in designs that ultimately connect us to the broad vistas of life's many landscapes.
Reading gives us the opportunity to appreciate those landscapes in all their variety.
It is remarkable that, whatever approach, method, or ideology is used to teach
reading, most students become proficient at it. For many students, successful
reading is assimilated into their experience quickly and with seeming smoothness.
Fortunate, the students for whom reading comes easily! For perhaps as many as 20%
of students, however, reading is not an automatic skill. Patterns of understanding
have to be systematically instilled so that the reader has the opportunity to
crack the alphabetic code. More and more, what we have learned is that connecting
these alphabetic symbols to specific sounds in order to create meaningful words
and phrases is a significant aspect of reading. There is considerable longitudinal
research to support that we all employ this skill every time we read. Without
this connection between the basic unit of sound and the alphabetic symbol, reading
does not occur for any of us (Liberman & Liberman, 1990). These basic units of sounds
contained within each word are called phonemes. Research has shown that understanding
of these phonemic units, more than any other factor, is a critical part of successful
reading. Phonemic awareness involves analyzing and combining the smallest units
of discernable sound (phonemes) in a variety of ways, in order to connect the
symbols (letters) which represent them, to specific meanings. The word "bat,"
for example, has three phonemes. The phonic approach would involve helping the
student discover the word produced when the /b/, /a/, and /t/ sounds are put
together. Throughout the history of
the teaching of reading, there has been a great debate between those who advocate
teaching reading through structured language approaches involving phonics, and
those who suggest that it is sufficient to grasp the relation of a whole word
to its meaning derived from some larger context (Chall, 1989). In the example
used above, the word "bat" might be learned through its placement
in a sentence such as, "We use a bat in baseball." For many years,
the trend in school systems has been to use this method, called the "whole
language" approach. Unfortunately, in schools
where the whole language approach is used to teach reading, simultaneous teaching
of explicit phonics is not always considered useful or necessary. Whether the
phonemic or the whole language approach is used, always we find that there is
a block of children who still do not learn to read. This suggests that school
systems have no systematic way for determining a satisfactory match between
the way which an individual child learns to read and the teaching approaches
used. In her book Beginning to Read, commentator Marilyn Adams (1990) suggests
that the phonemic and whole language approaches do not have to be mutually exclusive
but can complement each other's strengths to the betterment of all readers.
Recently, it appears that there is a movement to follow her sage advice. Also,
long-term research is being conducted through the National Institute of Child
Health and Human Development (NICHD) grants to determine how to match different
types of learners and effective teaching approaches. Several of these approaches
directly reflect our knowledge of phonemic awareness. The validity of the phonemic
approach is supported by considerable empirical data spanning more than 20 years.
Much of the research has been sponsored by NICHD through projects such as the
Dyslexia Program Projects in the late 1970s and the Learning Disability Research
Centers in the late 1980s. Working independently and on different projects,
these centers have achieved convergent results that are extremely compelling
(Moats & Lyon, 1993). In fact, the importance of phonological awareness
in learning to read is one of the few aspects of reading supported by such substantial,
long-term research. This research has demonstrated that a significant number
of children (15 to 20%) do not learn to read successfully unless they receive
direct instruction in phonological awareness. Therefore, it is imperative that,
whatever approach is used to teach reading skills, the needs of this population
must not be ignored. We do not know precisely
why acquisition of phonemic awareness is delayed in some students. We do know
that certain kinds of language activities for the pre-academic child make a
substantial difference. For example, rhyming games can have an important effect
(Bradley & Bryant, 1985). Children who have difficulty with rhyming often
seem to have difficulty learning to read. Therefore, utilizing rhyming games
and songs with young children can assist in identifying those children who may
have difficulty reading later, so that useful interventions can be made to strengthen
their skills. Keith Stanovitch (1993) outlines several activities that enhance
phonemic awareness: -- Phonemic deletion: What
word would be left if the /k/ sound were taken away from cat?
-- Word-to-word matching: Do pen and pipe begin with the same sound?
-- Blending: What word would we have if we put these sounds together: /s/, /a/,
/t/?
-- Sound isolation: What is the first sound in rose?
-- Phoneme segmentation: What sounds do you hear in the word hot?
-- Phoneme counting: How many sounds do you hear in the word cake?
-- Deleted phoneme: What sound do you hear in meet that is missing in eat?
-- Odd word out: What word starts with a different sound: bag, nine, beach,
bike?
-- Sound-to-word matching: Is there a /k/ in bike? Activities of this kind
can be fun and interesting to all children. To those for whom increased phonemic
awareness is essential, they are a godsend. Nevertheless, even if we were to
achieve a perfect record of teaching phonemic awareness, there will still be
a significant number of individuals who will have difficulty with reading and
other language tasks. Researchers in the medical and educational fields are
pursuing avenues for understanding the root causes of these problems. It is
hoped that such understanding will bring about some additional ways of presenting
reading, so that yet another subset of poor readers can be helped. With phonological awareness,
we have the opportunity to establish modes of teaching that are based on solid
research. Annual data supplied by the U.S. Department of Education (1995) show
that more than 50% of school-age youngsters being provided special education
services have learning disabilities and that, of these, more than 80% manifest
their difficulties in reading and language. It is clear that a major effort
in teaching phonological awareness at the earliest possible opportunity will
have a significant impact on reducing the number of individuals who will require
special services. A major impediment to implementing
a phonemic approach is the poor level of phonemic awareness among teachers who
teach reading and among teachers in general. This lack of awareness is reported
in a major survey by Louisa Moats (1994), who found that teachers often were
aware of their lack of knowledge and earnestly sought greater understanding,
but they had received little training. Moats' findings suggest that shifting
our approach to teaching reading means training teachers in phonological awareness.
Teachers who do not understand the structural basis of language will have little
success in teaching it or in perceiving the difficulties with language that
some children have. Anderson et al. (1985),
in their seminal report Becoming a Nation of Readers, alerted the nation to
the need for explicit phonic teaching. As early as 1985, they had observed a
decline in reading scores among selected groups of children. The report, perhaps
overshadowed by other more startling calls for school reform, never received
the attention it merited. In the intervening years, little has changed, and
test scores continue to decline. The nation has poured millions of tax dollars
into research that supports phonological approaches, yet little has changed
in the school systems. Emphasizing phonological awareness in teaching reading
is an approach that appears to match the method to our body of knowledge. Phonological
awareness has the potential to unravel the mysteries of reading to countless
thousands of individuals, and to protect their well-being as well as that of
the nation. The phonemic approach to reading is an area where we actually have
well-documented tools. We need to use them.
ABOUT THE AUTHOR: As an educator and advocate,
William Ellis contributed a great deal to the learning disabilities field. During
his career, Mr. Ellis chaired numerous national symposia, edited several books,
and published many articles on reading and learning. Prior to his death in 1995,
he served as the Director of the National Center for Learning Disabilities and
as Executive Editor of Their World, an annual publication of NCLD. Most of all,
he was a man of tremendous compassion.Bill wrote this article for NICHCY in
the last months of his life. We at NICHCY are proud to offer this work to the
field.
National Information Center for
Children and Youth with Disabilities
P.O. Box 1492
Washington, DC 20013
1-800-695-0285 (Voice/TT)
E-mail: nichcy@aed.org
Web site: http://www.nichcy.org/This information is copyright
free. Readers are encouraged to copy and share it, but please credit the National
Information Center for Children and Youth with Disabilities (NICHCY). |